Abstract
Abstract: The question of who should act, and how often, is critical for cultures and the regulation of social behavior. The vertical/horizontal dimension of culture describes the relative valuing of hierarchy versus equality. In a horizontal culture valuing equality, responsibility for action is more widely distributed than in a vertical culture valuing hierarchy. The relation between this cultural dimension and general attitudes towards action and inaction was tested with a large-scale survey of respondents from 19 nations. A multi-level model indicated that liking for action was especially associated with horizontality--the valuing of equality. Although values can generally be expressed through various compatible actions, horizontality (valuing equality) entails endorsing distributed responsibility for action and its outcomes, promoting general favorability towards action. In contrast, verticality includes countervailing components that discourage action by promoting norms that constrain who should act in accordance to status.
Resumen: La cuestión de quién debe actuar y con qué frecuencia es crítica para las culturas y la regulación del comportamiento social. La dimensión vertical / horizontal de la cultura describe la valoración relativa de la jerarquía versus la igualdad. En una cultura horizontal que valora la igualdad, la responsabilidad de la acción se distribuye más ampliamente que en una jerarquía de valores verticales. La relación entre esta dimensión cultural y las actitudes generales hacia la acción y la inacción fue probada con una encuesta a gran escala de encuestados de 19 naciones. Un modelo multinivel indica que el gusto por la acción se asocia especialmente con la horizontalidad - la valoración de la igualdad. Aunque los valores pueden expresarse generalmente a través de diversas acciones compatibles, la horizontalidad (valoración de la igualdad) implica respaldar la responsabilidad distribuida de la acción y sus resultados, promoviendo la acción. En cambio, la verticalidad incluye componentes compensatorios que desalientan la acción y promueve normas que limitan quién debe actuar de acuerdo al estatus
References
Albarracín, D., Handley, I.M., Noguchi, K., McCulloch, K.C., Li, H., Leeper, J., Brown, R.D.,
Earl, A., & Hart, W.P. (2008). Increasing and decreasing motor and cognitive output: a model of general action and inaction goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 510–23.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models
using Eigin and S4. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
DiMaggio, P. (1997). Culture and cognition. Annual Review of Sociology, 23, 263–287.
Fazio, R. H., Sherman, S. J., & Herr, P. M. (1982). The feature-positive effect in the self-
perception process: Does not doing matter as much as doing? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 42(3), 404–411. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.42.3.404
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford U. Press.
Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453–66.
Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2010). The implicit “go”: masked action cues directly
mobilize mental effort. Psychological Science, 21, 1389–93.
Hofstede, G. (1984). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hong, Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A
dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55,
-720.
Ireland, M. E., Hepler, J., Li, H., & Albarracín, D. (2014). Neuroticism and attitudes towards
action in 19 countries. Journal of Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12099
Jenkins, H. M., & Sainsbury, R. S. (1969). The development of stimulus control through
differential reinforcement. In N. J. Mackintosh & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Funamental issues
in associative learning. Halifax, Nova Scotia, CA: Dalhousie UP.
John, O. P. & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality:
Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). New York, NY: Guilford.
Kitayama, S. & Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future
directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 419-49.
Laran, J. (2010). The influence of information processing goal pursuit on postdecision affect and
behavioral intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 16–28.
Levine, R. V., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). The pace of life in 31 Countries. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 30, 178–205.
Markus, H.R. & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self : Implications for cognition, emotion,
and Motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.
McCulloch, K.C., Li, H., Hong, S., & Albarracin, D. (2012). Naïve definitions of action
and inaction: The continuum, spread, and valence of behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 227–234.
Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (2002). Culture and cognition. In D. Medin & H. Pashler (Eds),
Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology, Vol 2.: Memory and cognitive processes.
New York, NY: Wiley & Sons.
Noguchi, K., Handley, I. M., & Albarracín, D. (2011). Participating in politics resembles
physical activity: General action patterns in international archives, United States archives, and experiments. Psychological Science, 22, 235–42.
Oyserman, D. & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects
of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311-342.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311
Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt
impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768–774.
Rinne, T., Steel, G. D., & Fairweather, J. (2011). Hofstede and Shane revisited: The role of
power distance and individualism in national-level innovation success. Cross-Cultural Research, 46, 91–108.
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2005). Individualism: A valid and important dimension
of cultural differences between nations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 17–31.
Schwartz, S. H. & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the universal content and structure of
values: Extensions and cross-cultural replications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 878–891.
Shavitt, S., Lalwani, A.K., Zhang, J., & Torelli, C. J. (2006). The horizontal/vertical
distinction in cross-cultural consumer research. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 325–356.
Singelis, T.M., Triandis, H.C., Bhawuk, D., & Gelfand, M.J. (1995) Horizontal and vertical
dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research 29, 240-275.
Spencer-Rogers, J., Srivastava, S., Boucher, H. C., English, T., Paletz, S. B., Wang, L., … Peng,
K. (2010). The dialectical self scale. Unpublished manuscript, University of California,
Santa Barbara.
Triandis, H. C. (1964). Cultural influences upon cognitive processes. In L. Berkiwitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 1-48). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Triandis, H.C. & Gelfand, M.J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical
individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 118-128.
Zell, E., Su, R., Li, H., Ho, M.-H. R., Hong, S., Kumkale, T., Rossier, J., Massoudi, K.,
Cai, H., Roccas, S., Arce-Michel, J., de Sousa, C., Diaz-Loving, R., Botero, M. M, Mannetti, L., Garcia, C., Carrera, P., Cabalero, A., Ikemi, M., Chan, D., Bernardo, A., Garcia, F., Brechan, I., Maio, G., & Albarracín, D. (2012). Cultural
Differences in Attitudes Toward Action and Inaction: The Role of Dialecticism. Social Psychological and Personality Science. doi:10.1177/1948550612468774
Zhang, Y., Winterich, K. P., & Mittal, V. (2010). Power Distance Belief and Impulsive Buying.
Journal of Marketing Research, 47, 945–954
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
- Authors retain copyright and grant the journal right of first publication, with the work [SPECIFY PERIOD OF TIME] after publication simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgment of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
- Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgment of its initial publication in this journal.
- Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).